The Importance of Fantasy

The following is an excerpt from a 1974 book chapter titled "A Little about Love", by Ellen Berscheid & Elaine Hatfield. (See: doi, scans, pages 358–359, 380.) The chapter is also cited by Dorothy Tennov on page 56 of Love and Limerence (note: page 282), in her discussion of the Romeo and Juliet effect.

A. The Importance of Fantasy

Researchers have generally assumed that it is the actual rewards which are exchanged during interpersonal contact which create liking. It seems doubtful, however, that people are so reality-bound.

When the lover closes his eyes and daydreams, he can summon up a flawless partner—a partner who instantaneously satisfies all his unspoken, conflicting, and fleeting desires. In fantasy he may receive unlimited reward or he may anticipate that he would receive unlimited reward were he ever to actually meet his ideal.

Compared to our grandiose fantasies, the level of reward we receive in our real interactions is severely circumscribed. As a consequence, sometimes the most extreme passion is aroused by partners who exist only in imagination or partners who are barely known. Reactions to real-life love objects often seem to be far more tepid.

Theorists interested only in liking, then, may possibly afford to focus entirely on the impact actual reward has on liking. In contrast, it seems likely that romantic love theorists will be forced to take into consideration both the rewards a lover receives in fantasy and the rewards he fantasizes he might receive in future interaction with the partner. (Further discussion of the importance of fantasy in generating passionate love is provided by Reik, 1944, and the current state of scientific knowledge of fantasy is reviewed by Klinger, 1971.)

Klinger, E. Structure and functions of fantasy. New York: Wiley, 1971.

Reik, T. A psychologist looks at love. New York: Farrar & Rinehart, 1944. 

Note then that the contemporary idea is that idealization (regarded as positive illusions) involves the reward system.

[T]he comparison between partners and non-close others may be facilitated and made more prominent by the processing of a partner’s positive characteristics by the caudate nucleus, while the dACC suppresses the perception of a partner’s negative features. At the same time, the vLPFC and dMPFC could be reducing the salience of attractive alternatives. Subsequently, these brain areas transmit signals to the vACC that may help differentiate information from potential partners over non-close others. Finally, as information is passed onto the OFC, the weighing of positive and negative information of a partner is redistributed, cementing biased subjective values (e.g., positive illusion) about the partner[.] (Song et al., 2019)

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

incurable romantics

How does Dorothy Tennov define limerence?

Limerence and anxious attachment